• Skip to main content
  • Skip to primary sidebar
  • Home
  • Categories
  • Authors
  • Print Versions
  • About
  • Masthead
    • 2022-2023
    • 2016-2017

The Bowdoin Review

Mirror Neurons and Empathy

Written by: Anneka Williams
Published on: November 3, 2018

You’re watching football on TV and wince at a particularly nasty tackle. Or you see someone getting a shot and rub your own arm, imagining the pain. You watch someone fall off their bike and cringe. All of these experiences, experiences where you react almost instinctively to other people’s experiences, may be due to mirror neurons.

In the 1980’s, Italian neuroscientist Giacomo Rizzolatti and a research team observed neurons firing in an area of the premotor cortex – also referred to as F5 – of macaque monkeys when the monkeys reached for a peanut. Intending to study how those neurons fired in response to various objects and actions, the researchers quickly observedthat the same neurons that fired when the monkey grasped a peanut also fired when the monkey watched a researcher grasp a peanut. The researchers were able to observe individual neuron responses to very specific actions and later dubbed their discovery “mirror neurons.” While harder to isolate single neurons in humans, scientists have still been able to find evidence of a mirror neuron system in human brains.

Though mirror neurons do not comprise all the neurons in the body and do not have minds of their own, science has found them to be incredibly complex and nuanced in their function. In an experiment with a teacup, Christian Keysers investigated the broader role mirror neurons may play in shaping how we understand other people’s emotions and intentions. Their experiment tried to determine whether mirror neurons were capable of distinguishing between various possible intentions associated with picking up a teacup. Keysers and his colleagues took fMRIs of their participants, as the participants watched two different videos. The first video showed a hand picking up a teacup that was situated in the midst of a messy table. The second video showed a hand picking up a teacup that sat by itself on a table with no external cues to suggest what purpose picking up the teacup was fulfilling. The data showed that mirror neurons were more active in response to the video that contained context (i.e. the messy table) rather than the contextless video, suggesting that there is a way in which these neurons can begin to recognize not only the actions of others, but also their intentions.

Many scientists have credited mirror neurons as the basis of empathy and as a key factor in allowing humans to interact in a complex social world. V.S. Ramachandran, a professor of neuroscience at the University of California San Diego, went so far as to call them the “basis of civilization” in his TED talk. While mirror neurons alone don’t allow us to effectively engage with the complex communities in which we live, Ramachandran makes a strong case for their importance, particularly with reference to their ability to allow us to empathize with others. In an interview with Greater Good Magazine, Ramachandran explained: “if I really and truly empathize with your pain, I need to experience it myself. That’s what the mirror neurons are doing, allowing me to empathize with your pain—saying, in effect, that person is experiencing the same agony and excruciating pain as you would if somebody were to poke you with a needle directly. That’s the basis of all empathy.” He went on to clarify that “[mirror neurons] enable me to see you as an intentional being, with purpose and intention.” In fact, the extent to which mirror neurons have implications for society may be best encapsulated in the proposition that mirror neuron dysfunction may be involved in autism. Ramachandran suggests that people with autism lack “empathy” and a “theory of mind,”and this may be due to abnormally functioning mirror neurons. Other research, however, has suggested that people with autism do feel empathy, they just express it differently.

It is important when listening to Ramachandran to recognize the assumption in his argument of the importance of mirror neurons to society – namely, that empathy is a key ingredient to civilized society. Whether or not society would exist without empathy is a whole other line of inquiry, but thinking of a society without empathy certainly suggests that empathy may in fact be more important than we think in terms of our ability to see other humans as, well, human.

Categories: ScienceTags: Neuroscience

Primary Sidebar

Recent Posts

  • Why South Africa Remains Unequal Thirty Years After Apartheid May 7, 2024
  • Skeptical of September February 8, 2024
  • Waterwheel February 7, 2024
  • Nineteen February 7, 2024
  • D.C.’s Most Expensive Retirement Home: Congress    February 7, 2024
  • Instagram

Archives

  • May 2024
  • February 2024
  • October 2023
  • April 2023
  • February 2023
  • December 2022
  • November 2022
  • October 2022
  • September 2022
  • May 2022
  • April 2022
  • February 2022
  • December 2021
  • November 2021
  • April 2021
  • March 2021
  • February 2021
  • December 2020
  • November 2020
  • October 2020
  • September 2020
  • April 2020
  • February 2020
  • January 2020
  • December 2019
  • November 2019
  • October 2019
  • May 2019
  • April 2019
  • March 2019
  • February 2019
  • January 2019
  • December 2018
  • November 2018
  • October 2018
  • August 2018
  • May 2018
  • April 2018
  • March 2018
  • February 2018
  • January 2018
  • December 2017
  • November 2017
  • October 2017
  • September 2017
  • June 2017
  • May 2017
  • April 2017
  • March 2017
  • February 2017
  • December 2016
  • November 2016
  • October 2016
  • May 2016
  • April 2016
  • March 2016
  • February 2016
  • January 2016
  • December 2015
  • November 2015
  • May 2015
  • April 2015
  • December 2014
  • October 2014
  • April 2014
  • March 2014
  • December 2013
  • November 2013
  • February 2012

Copyright © 2025 · The Bowdoin Review - A voice on campus for politics, society, and culture.