• Skip to primary navigation
  • Skip to main content
  • Skip to primary sidebar
  • Skip to footer

Bowdoin Science Journal

  • Home
  • About
    • Our Mission
    • Our Staff
  • Sections
    • Biology
    • Chemistry and Biochemistry
    • Math and Physics
    • Computer Science and Technology
    • Environmental Science and EOS
    • Honors Projects
    • Psychology and Neuroscience
  • Contact Us
  • Fun Links
  • Subscribe

Psychology and Neuroscience

The Contraceptive Brain Drain: How Birth Control Alters Women’s Brains

April 2, 2023 by Divya Bhargava '26

There are millions of women taking steroids every day. But how is this possible? Are they just getting really buff? It feels like we always hear stories about how performance-enhancing drugs, namely steroids, are giving world-class athletes the boost they need to beat out their competition. But women across the globe are taking steroids every day as well, in the form of hormonal birth control. Despite their widespread use, side effects of hormonal contraceptives are largely unstudied, or have been until recently. In the last ten years, several studies have come out about the effect of taking a daily dose of steroids on women’s brains and mental health, which until now has been a severely neglected area where lack of knowledge affects millions of people worldwide. 

People take hormonal birth control, or hormonal contraceptives, for a myriad of reasons, from the obvious (preventing pregnancy) to the not-so-obvious (lessening iron deficiency) and everything in between. This type of medication simply refers to methods of pregnancy prevention that act on the endocrine system. The endocrine system controls growth, development, metabolism, and reproduction via signaling molecules called hormones. Two hormones in particular, estrogen and progesterone, control the menstrual cycle and are therefore the major components of hormonal birth control. Types of hormonal contraceptives come in many forms including the pill, the patch, the implant, injections, and hormonal intrauterine devices or IUDs, but despite the wide variety in the forms this medication takes, all of them contain one or both of these two hormones. As steroids, both estrogen and progesterone affect other body systems besides the reproductive system.

To study the impacts of taking a daily dose of steroids on other areas of the body, specifically the brain, Dr. Belinda Pletzer and her colleagues conducted a study in 2010. The brain is particularly susceptible to change due to an influx of synthetic hormones because it contains a very high quantity of hormone receptors. The brain needs to act as a “sponge” for these molecules since it plays an important role in creating the appropriate responses in the rest of the body. Pletzer’s study investigated how the sponginess of the brain would affect changes in its structure by comparing images of the brains of adult men, adult women during different stages of their menstrual cycle, and adult women taking hormonal contraceptives. To perform this comparison they used a technique called voxel-based morphology on MRIs of study participants (Pletzer et al., 2010). Voxel-based morphology measures differences in the concentration of tissue and the size and shape of different areas of the brain.

Overall, they found that women taking hormonal birth control had smaller areas of gray matter, or areas of the brain that have a high concentration of the cell bodies of nerve cells, when compared to “naturally cycling women” in both their follicular and luteal menstrual phases (Figure 1). Pletzer’s study also found interesting gendered differences in gray matter volume. While men had greater gray matter overall, the volume of gray matter in the prefrontal cortex, the pre-and postcentral gyri, and the supramarginal gyrus of both naturally cycling women and women taking hormonal contraceptives was higher than the volume of gray matter in these areas in men (Figure 2). These areas are involved in decision-making and problem-solving, controlling motor function, and emotional responses. However, the higher amount of gray matter in women in these areas was overshadowed by the larger volume of gray matter in men in the hippocampus, hypothalamus, parahippocampal and fusiform gyri, putamen, pallidum, amygdala, and temporal regions of the brain during the early follicular phase (A), mid-luteal phase (B), and in women taking hormonal birth control (C) (Figure 2). Many of these areas of reduced gray matter are ones of high importance for neurophysical ability and mental health.

Additionally, a study done by Rush University Medical Center showed an association between higher levels of gray matter and better cognitive function (“Everyday Activities Associated with More Gray Matter in Brains of Older Adults”). These findings suggest that taking birth control, and the associated decrease in gray matter, could be directly causing some of the symptoms women on hormonal contraceptives experience, such as brain fog, mood changes, and even anxiety and depression. For example, a smaller hypothalamus, one of the areas of decreased gray matter, is associated with heightened irritability and depression symptoms (“Study Finds Key Brain Region Smaller in Birth Control Pill Users”). Pletzer’s research and the work of others after her on the impact of birth control on structures of the brain represent important first steps in proving a causative relationship between birth control, symptoms associated with it, and structural changes in the brain.

Although this research has made some crucial preliminary steps into researching how taking a daily dose of steroids affects the brains of women taking hormonal contraceptives, the highly complex nature of the brain and its relationship with the regulation of the rest of the body means that further research is necessary. The sheer number of people that this issue affects means that it is essential to continue researching the impacts of this widely used drug. More importantly, knowing the potentially serious negative side effects enables millions of people to make more informed decisions concerning their health and their bodies.

 

Works Cited

Rush University Medical Center. (2018, February 14). Everyday activities associated with more gray matter in brains of older adults: Study measured amount of lifestyle physical activity such as house work, dog walking and gardening. ScienceDaily. Retrieved March 11, 2023 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2018/02/180214093828.htm.

Lewis, C. A., Kimmig, A. C. S., Zsido, R. G., Jank, A., Derntl, B., & Sacher, J. (2019). Effects of hormonal contraceptives on mood: a focus on emotion recognition and reactivity, reward processing, and stress response. Current psychiatry reports, vol. 21, no.11, 2019, p 115. PubMed Central, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-019-1095-z.

Meyer, Craig H., Kinsley, Elizabeth A. “Women’s Brains on Steroids.” Scientific American, https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/womens-brains-on-steroids/. Accessed 7 Mar. 2023.

Nemoto, Kiyotaka. “[Understanding Voxel-Based Morphometry].” Brain and Nerve = Shinkei Kenkyu No Shinpo, vol. 69, no. 5, May 2017, pp. 505–11. PubMed, https://doi.org/10.11477/mf.1416200776.

Pletzer, Belinda, et al. “Menstrual Cycle and Hormonal Contraceptive Use Modulate Human Brain Structure.” Brain Research, vol. 1348, Aug. 2010, pp. 55–62. ScienceDirect, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.06.019.

Sharma, Rupali, et al. “Use of the Birth Control Pill Affects Stress Reactivity and Brain Structure and Function.” Hormones and Behavior, vol. 124, Aug. 2020, p. 104783. ScienceDirect, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104783.

“Study Finds Key Brain Region Smaller in Birth Control Pill Users.” ScienceDaily, https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2019/12/191204090819.htm. Accessed 7 Mar. 2023.

Filed Under: Biology, Psychology and Neuroscience, Science Tagged With: Biology, Birth control, Medicine, Women's health

Neurogenesis in the Adult Brain

November 6, 2022 by Alena Lemeshova '26

Have you ever heard that the brain’s development stops during childhood? Did it ever make you wonder how you still can learn to speak a foreign language, ride a bike, or solve calculus problems? While you might not have gotten a chance to do any of those things when you were little, you can learn them at almost any point in your life. How? Researchers have been hunting for an answer for the last seventy years.

Neurogenesis is the process by which new neurons develop. For a long time, scientists believed that neurogenesis occurs only during the prenatal period, with children being born with a finite amount of neurons. However, research in the 1960s suggested that new cells develop from stem cells in the brain even during adulthood, though at much slower rates (Altman, 1965).

New neurons come from divided neural stem cells, which travel to particular brain regions and mature into fully functioning neurons. Scientists track these migrations by adding chemicals inside the progenitor – “parent” – stem cells and matching the corresponding chemical signatures after the divisions. Originally, animals such as non-human primates and mice were the main test subjects, but we now have a considerable amount of data on human neurogenesis. Scientists have recorded the occurrence of human neurogenesis in the ninth decade of some test subjects’ lives (Anacker, 2017).

The primary region of neurogenesis is the hippocampus, a structure responsible for learning and memory – and partially for moods (Gage, 2021). It is highly vulnerable to neurological disorders such as major depressive disorder and generalized anxiety disorder, which diminish the number of functioning neurons inside it. Understanding more about neurogenesis might help scientists develop better treatments for these conditions by reversing their effects and maintaining a stable number of cells in the hippocampus. 

A more recent idea that research introduced is that neurogenesis supports not only the number of neural cells in the brain but also optimizes the neural networks they form. A research group found that newly generated granule cells – the smallest and the most common type of neurons – promoted neural sparsity (leaving only important neurons and eliminating all the ineffective ones) in the hippocampus, allowing for better and faster information processing. Thus, the brain seems to have more tools to rewire itself, maintaining the balance between the number of cells and their effective organization (McHugh, 2022).

Despite dozens of years of research, neurogenesis might be one of the oldest topics in neuroscience which has not still disclosed its secrets. At the same time, the prospects it opens for medical and scientific fields are enormous. Maybe, one day, with the secrets of neurogenesis uncovered, humans will finally see the world that has a treatment available for any neuropsychological disorder.

 

References

Altman, J., & Das, G. D. (1965). Post-Natal Origin of Microneurones in the Rat Brain. Nature, 207(5000), 953–956. https://doi.org/10.1038/207953a0

Anacker, C., & Hen, R. (2017). Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and cognitive flexibility—Linking memory and mood. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(6), 335–346. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.45

Gage, F. H. (2019). Adult neurogenesis in mammals. Science, 364(6443), 827–828. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aav6885

Gage, F. H. (2021). Adult neurogenesis in neurological diseases. Science, 374(6571), 1049–1050. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abm7468

McHugh, S. B., Lopes-dos-Santos, V., Gava, G. P., Hartwich, K., Tam, S. K. E., Bannerman, D. M., & Dupret, D. (2022). Adult-born dentate granule cells promote hippocampal population sparsity. Nature Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-022-01176-5

Filed Under: Psychology and Neuroscience

The Kleptomania Connection between Serotonin and Stealing

April 15, 2022 by Luv Kataria '24

Although many people steal in response to economic hardship, either perceived or actual, some individuals only steal to satisfy a powerful urge. These individuals may have an impulse control disorder known as kleptomania. People with kleptomania experience a sense of relief from stealing, so they steal to get rid of their anxiety (Talih, 2011). The prevalence of kleptomania in the U.S. is estimated to be 6 people per 1000, which is equivalent to more than 1.5 million kleptomaniacs in the U.S. population ​​(Aboujaoude et al., 2004).

What exactly causes this impulse to steal? Kleptomania has a range of biological, psychological, and sociological risk factors. One of the main biological factors has to do with neurotransmitters, such as serotonin (Sulthana, 2015). Serotonin plays an important role in our bodies, contributing to emotions and judgment, and low serotonin levels have been linked to impulsive and aggressive behaviors (Williams, 2002). The serotonin system is also thought to be involved in “increased cognitive impulsivity,” as has been observed in individuals with a higher number of kleptomania symptoms (Ascher & Levounis, 2014).

Throughout the nervous system, serotonin transporters (SERT) take up serotonin that is released from neurons (Rudnick, 2007). These transporters can also be found on blood platelets and take up serotonin from the blood plasma (Mercado & Kilic, 2010). We can study these particular transporters to better understand the levels of serotonin in one’s blood and how that relates to their level of impulsiveness.

A 2010 study looked into the relationship between the platelet serotonin transporter, impulsivity, and gender. They found that while women were, in general, more impulsive than men, there was only a positive correlation between the number of transporters and impulsivity in men. This means that higher amounts of platelet serotonin transporters and lower levels of serotonin are related to more impulsivity in men, but not in women. It was also found that higher amounts of SERT transporters were linked to more “aggressive” behaviors. The authors came to the conclusion that, even though women were found to display more impulsivity than men, serotonin plays a larger role in impulsivity with men than it does with women (Marazziti et al., 2010).

Understanding the relationship between serotonin and impulsivity with kleptomania has helped pioneer specific treatments, including Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors (SSRIs). Impulsivity is linked to low levels of serotonin, so SSRIs fix this by limiting the reuptake of serotonin through the blockage of serotonin transporters, leading to the buildup of serotonin in the synapse (Sulthana, 2015). There is no cure for kleptomania, but SSRIs help to control the impulse to steal. 

Overall, kleptomania is a secretive disorder, for which many people don’t seek help due to the legal system and the social stigma around theft. Thus, very little is known about what causes kleptomania, but trying to understand it through its link with neurotransmitters has uncovered potential causes and helped develop treatments. 

 

References

Ascher, M. S., & Levounis, P. (Eds.). (2014). The behavioral addictions. American Psychiatric Publishing.

Aboujaoude, E., Gamel, N., & Koran, L. M. (2004a). Overview of kleptomania and phenomenological description of 40 patients. Primary Care Companion to The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 6(6), 244–247. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC535651/ 

Marazziti, D., Baroni, S., Masala, I., Golia, F., Consoli, G., Massimetti, G., Picchetti, M., Dell’Osso, M. C., Giannaccini, G., Betti, L., Lucacchini, A., & Ciapparelli, A. (2010). Impulsivity, gender, and the platelet serotonin transporter in healthy subjects. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 6, 9–15. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2951061/ 

Mercado, C. P., & Kilic, F. (2010). Molecular mechanisms of SERT in platelets: regulation of plasma serotonin levels. Molecular interventions, 10(4), 231–241. https://doi.org/10.1124/mi.10.4.6 

Rudnick, G. (2007). Sert, serotonin transporter. In S. J. Enna & D. B. Bylund (Eds.), XPharm: The Comprehensive Pharmacology Reference (pp. 1–6). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-008055232-3.60442-8

Sulthana, N., Singh, M., & Vijaya, K. (2015). Kleptomania-the Compulsion to Steal. Am. J. Pharm. Tech. Res, 5(3). 

Talih, F. R. (2011b). Kleptomania and potential exacerbating factors. Innovations in Clinical Neuroscience, 8(10), 35–39. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3225132/ 

Williams, Julie. Pyromania, Kleptomania, and Other Impulse-Control Disorders. Enslow, 2002. 

Filed Under: Biology, Psychology and Neuroscience, Science Tagged With: kleptomania, serotonin, SERT

Mimicking the Human Brain: The Role of Heterogeneity in Artificial Intelligence

April 10, 2022 by Jenna Albanese '24

Picture this: you’re in the passenger seat of a car, weaving through an urban metropolis – say New York City. As expected, you see plenty of people: those who are rushed, lingering, tourists, locals, old, young, et cetera. But let’s zoom in: take just about any one of those individuals in the city, and you will find 86 billion nerve cells, or neurons, in their brain carrying them through daily life. For comparison, this means that the number of neurons in the human brain is about ten thousand times the number of residents in New York City.

But let’s zoom in even further: each one of those 86 billion neurons in the brain is ever-so-slightly different from one another. For example, while some neurons work extremely quickly in making decisions that guide basic processes in the brain, others work more slowly, basing their decisions off surrounding neurons’ activity. This difference in decision-making time among our neurons is called heterogeneity. Previously, researchers were unsure of heterogeneity’s importance in our lives, but its existence was certain. This is just one example of the almost incomprehensible detail of the brain that makes human thinking so complex, and even difficult to fully understand for modern researchers.

Now, let’s zoom in again, but this time not on the person’s brain. Instead, let’s zoom into the cell phone this individual might have in their pocket or their hand. While a cell phone does not function exactly the same as the human brain, aspects of the device are certainly modeled after human thinking. Virtual assistants, like Siri or Cortana, for instance, compose responses to general inquiries that resemble human interaction.

This type of highly advanced digital experience is the result of artificial intelligence. Since the 1940s, elements of artificial intelligence have been modeled after features of the human brain,  fashioned as a neural network composed of nodes, some serving as inputs and others as outputs. The nodes are comparable to brain cells, and they communicate with each other through a series of algorithms to produce outputs. However, in these technological brain models, every node is typically modeled in the same way in terms of the time they take to respond to a given situation (Science Daily 2021). This is quite unlike the human brain, where heterogeneity ensures that each neuron responds to stimuli at different speeds. But does this even matter? Do intricate qualities of the brain like heterogeneity really make a difference in our thinking, or in digital functioning if incorporated into artificial intelligence?

The short answer is yes, at least in the case of heterogeneity. Researchers have recently investigated how heterogeneity influences an artificial neural network’s performance on visual and auditory information classification tasks. In the study, each neural network had a different “time constant,” which is how long the cells take to respond to a situation given the responses of nearby cells. In essence, the researchers varied the heterogeneity of the artificial neural networks. The results were astonishing: once heterogeneity was introduced, the artificial neural networks completed the tasks more efficiently and accurately. The strongest result revealed a 15-20% improvement on auditory tasks as soon as heterogeneity was introduced to the artificial neural network (Perez-Nieves et al. 2021).

This result indicates that heterogeneity helps us think systematically, improve our task performance, and learn in changing conditions (Perez-Nieves et al. 2021). So perhaps it would be advantageous to incorporate heterogeneity into standard artificial intelligence models. With this change, technology’s way of “thinking” will come one step closer to functioning like a human brain, adopting a similar level of complexity and intricacy.

So, why does this matter? If parts of artificial intelligence are modeled closer and closer to how the human brain works, real-world benefits abound, and we’re talking on a level grander than virtual assistants. One prominent example is in head and neck cancer prognosis. Clinical predictors of head and neck cancer prognosis include factors like age, pathological findings, HPV status, and tobacco and alcohol consumption (Chinnery et al. 2020). With a multitude of factors at play, physicians spend excessive amounts of time analyzing head and neck cancer patients’ lifestyles in order to deduce an accurate prognosis. Alternatively, artificial intelligence could be used to model this complex web of factors for these cancer patients, and physicians’ time could be spent on other endeavors.

This type of clinical application is still far from implementation, but remains in sight for modern researchers. As the brain is further explored and understood, more and more of the elements that comprise advanced human thinking can be incorporated into technology. Now, put yourself in the shoes of our New York City passerby: how would you feel if the small cell phone in your pocket was just as intelligent and efficient as the 86 billion neurons in your head? How about if that cell phone solved problems like you do and thought like you think, in essence serving as a smaller version of your own brain? It is almost unfathomable! Yet, by harnessing heterogeneity, researchers have come one step closer toward realizing this goal.

 

References

Chinnery, T., Arifin, A., Tay, K. Y., Leung, A., Nichols, A. C., Palma, D. A., Mattonen, S. A., & Lang, P. (2020). Utilizing artificial intelligence for head and neck cancer outcomes prediction from imaging. Canadian Association of Radiologists Journal, 72(1), 73–85. https://doi.org/10.1177/0846537120942134.

Perez-Nieves, N., Leung, V. C. H., Dragotti, P. L., & Goodman, D. F. M. (2021). Neural heterogeneity promotes robust learning. Nature Communications, 12(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-26022-3. 

ScienceDaily. (2021, October 6). Brain cell differences could be key to learning in humans and ai. ScienceDaily. Retrieved February 27, 2022, from https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2021/10/211006112626.htm.  

Filed Under: Computer Science and Tech, Psychology and Neuroscience Tagged With: AI, heterogeneity, neural network

When We Fall Asleep

December 5, 2021 by Grant Griesman '24

When our bodies shut down at night, our brains transport us into strange, convoluted alternate realities. Dreams range from the mundane to the fantastical, from classrooms to castles. Despite the sheer absurdity of many dreams, they always feel real. But where do dreams even come from, and why do we have them?

Definitions of a dream range from the generous “subjective experience during sleep” to the more specific “immersive spatiotemporal hallucination” (Siclari et al., 2020). Taken either way, dreams are characterized by increased blood flow to regions of the brain called the amygdala, hippocampus, and anterior cingulate cortex. The significant role that these regions play in regulating our emotions may explain the intense emotional aspect of many dreams (Schwartz & Maquet, 2002). 

There are five stages of sleep. The first four stages are collectively categorized as non-Rapid Eye Movement, or NREM, sleep. Accordingly, the fifth stage is referred to as the REM stage. During REM sleep, our eyeballs flit back and forth underneath our eyelids, our muscles are paralyzed to prevent self-injury from dream enactment, and our brain activity reflects that of wakefulness (Siclari et al., 2020). Although dreams are more common in REM sleep, recent research has shown that shorter and less bizarre dreams occur during NREM sleep as well (Nielsen, 2000).

It seems like something as peculiar as dreaming should have a distinct purpose. However, the exact function of dreams is still unknown. One theory speculates that dreams are simply a byproduct of other brain activity, such as memory consolidation, that occurs during sleep. Sigmund Freud, oft-considered the father of psychology, believed that dreams allowed for the disguised fulfillment of the sexual and aggressive desires of the id. According to Freud, the id is the component of our personality that lies below our consciousness and drives primitive, aggressive desires. Other theories suggest that dreaming is evolutionarily advantageous because it allows us to practice behaviors important to our survival in our sleep, preparing us for the same events in wakefulness. These behaviors include hunting, mating, responding to threats, and socializing (Siclari et al., 2020).

Some people seem to remember their dreams every night, while others claim to never dream at all. Dream recall averages at about one dream a week, but this varies widely. Practices such as keeping a dream journal and setting an alarm during a period of likely REM sleep improve recall.

 Recall is inherently easier with nightmares. By definition, nightmares cause awakening, while “bad dreams” contain similar emotionally troubling content but do not induce awakening (Robert & Zadra, 2014). There is evidence for a genetic predisposition to nightmares (Hublin et al., 1999).

Lucid dreams are a fascinating type of REM dreaming in which the individual is aware they are dreaming and may even be able to control the dream. Lucid dreams activate brain areas usually associated with insight and agency in wakefulness (Dresler et al., 2012). They also elicit the same eye movements and respiration patterns. For example, when asked to dive into a pool in their lucid dream, subjects briefly stopped breathing — as if they were underwater. The perception of time is also similar; counting from 0 to 10 in a lucid dream takes about as long as it does in real life. Lucid dreams provide particularly valuable insights into the mechanisms of dreaming because the dreamer can communicate with the researcher through pre-determined eye movements (Erlacher et al., 2014).

So what happens when we miss out on REM sleep and REM dreams? Unfortunately, modern society gives us plenty of chances to find out. Substances, especially alcohol and marijuana, decrease the time we spend in REM sleep. Medications such as benzodiazepines, antidepressants, and, ironically, sleeping pills also decrease REM sleep. Furthermore, exposure to artificial light before bed and the use of an alarm clock limit REM sleep. Collectively, the impact of these behaviors can hinder immune function, memory consolidation, and mood regulation (Naiman, 2017). 

Despite everything that scientists have discovered about dreams, there is still much about them that remains a mystery. Recently, researchers have been trying to interpret the content of dreams by using brain scans and machine learning to decode certain patterns of brain activity (Horikawa et al., 2013). For now, however, we can only take what we do know and marvel at the rest. Every night brings its own all-expenses-paid adventure into another reality.

References

Dresler, M., Wehrle, R., Spoormaker, V. I., Koch, S. P., Holsboer, F., Steiger, A., Obrig, H., Sämann, P. G., & Czisch, M. (2012). Neural Correlates of Dream Lucidity Obtained from Contrasting Lucid versus Non-Lucid REM Sleep: A Combined EEG/fMRI Case Study. Sleep, 35(7), 1017–1020. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.1974

Erlacher, D., Schädlich, M., Stumbrys, T., & Schredl, M. (2014). Time for actions in lucid dreams: Effects of task modality, length, and complexity. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1013. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01013

Horikawa, T., Tamaki, M., Miyawaki, Y., & Kamitani, Y. (2013). Neural Decoding of Visual Imagery During Sleep. Science, 340(6132), 639–642.

Hublin, C., Kaprio, J., Partinen, M., & Koskenvuo, M. (1999). Nightmares: Familial aggregation and association with psychiatric disorders in a nationwide twin cohort. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 88(4), 329–336. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19990820)88:4<329::AID-AJMG8>3.0.CO;2-E

Naiman, R. (2017). Dreamless: The silent epidemic of REM sleep loss. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1406(1), 77–85. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13447

Nielsen, T. A. (2000). A review of mentation in REM and NREM sleep: “Covert” REM sleep as a possible reconciliation of two opposing models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(6), 851–866. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0000399X

Robert, G., & Zadra, A. (2014). Thematic and Content Analysis of Idiopathic Nightmares and Bad Dreams. Sleep, 37(2), 409–417. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.3426

Schwartz, S., & Maquet, P. (2002). Sleep imaging and the neuro-psychological assessment of dreams. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(1), 23–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01818-0

Siclari, F., Valli, K., & Arnulf, I. (2020). Dreams and nightmares in healthy adults and in patients with sleep and neurological disorders. The Lancet Neurology, 19(10), 849–859. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(20)30275-1

Filed Under: Psychology and Neuroscience, Science Tagged With: dreams, REM, sleep

Toxin Therapy

March 1, 2021 by Joanna Lin '22

While the growth of mold on fruits and vegetables forgotten in the fridge is not an atypical occurrence, lethal spores slowly sprouting in improperly preserved or fermented foods lead to more than a smelly fridge. The Clostridium botulinum bacterium produces deadly botulinum toxins (BoNT) that destroy proteins critical for the release of acetylcholine, the neurotransmitter primarily responsible for muscular function, into the neuromuscular synapse. The simple bacterium may be microscopic, but its ability to inhibit signals in the muscular network are potent and can induce irreversible paralysis. 

Clostridium botulinum produces lethal toxins that disrupt muscular contraction.
Photo credits: Dr. Phil Luton/Science Photo Library/Corbis

Exocytosis, the release of neurotransmitters into the synapse via vesicle-membrane fusion, primarily requires the complete assembly of three proteins: SNAP-25, syntaxin, and synaptobrevin. The bridging of these proteins between the vesicle and the plasma membrane are crucial for neurotransmitter release. Once the vesicles bind to the plasma membrane, neurotransmitters are released into the synapse and the action potential signals from the presynaptic neurons are sent to the postsynaptic muscle fibers. When these signals are blocked, however, muscle contractions are inhibited — initiating paralysis. 


Several types of botulinum toxins target critical proteins for exocytosis and inhibit the release of acetylcholine.

The structure of BoNT allows it to penetrate neurons and cleave the proteins that transfer the signals for movement. The toxins have 2 subunits, a light and heavy chain, which work together to penetrate the neuron and wreak havoc. The heavy chain dictates which neurons are affected by the toxins by strongly binding to the external membrane. They facilitate the entry of the light chain into the cytoplasm of synaptic terminals, which then disrupts exocytosis by snipping the critical proteins for vesicle-membrane fusion. The structure of the light chain determines which proteins are cleaved. The toxins ultimately causes a paralytic effect by inhibiting membrane fusion of vesicles and acetylcholine release at neuromuscular junctions.

The extreme potency and lethality of botulinum toxins makes them potentially fatal bioweapons. Small amounts of BoNT can be deadly, where “a single gram of crystalline toxin, evenly dispersed and inhaled, can kill more than one million people.” The lethal dose for humans orally is estimated to be 30 ng and by inhalation 0.80 to 0.90 µg. An estimate of only 39.2 g of pure BoNT could eradicate humankind. While the inhibition of neurotransmitter release is irreversible, the paralytic effects are felt in full force by four to seven days after exposure. The long latency of effects can delay alarm and medical treatment. While some paralytic effects may be mediated by the growth of new nerve terminals and synaptic connections, these recovery processes can take up to months.

The lethality of these toxins have been harnessed for a range of purposes, from cosmetic procedures to treatments for movement disorders. BoNT is colloquially well-known as Botox, the drug commonly used to smooth facial wrinkles and enhance a youthful appearance. Beyond the surface, Botox has also been FDA-approved to treat chronic migraines, excessive sweating, and several other medical conditions. Other applications are under investigation, but the botulinum toxins have been found to reduce tremors, tics, muscle spasms, and other movement disorders that derive from debilitating neurological diseases.

The potential uses of these toxins may enhance the quality of life for many people. While the use of deadly botulinum toxins for medical treatments may seem unorthodox, these compounds have proven to be incredibly versatile in their application.

Filed Under: Chemistry and Biochemistry, Psychology and Neuroscience Tagged With: BoNT, C. botulinum, Clostridium botulinum, neurobiology

Primary Sidebar

CATEGORY CLOUD

Biology Chemistry and Biochemistry Computer Science and Tech Environmental Science and EOS Math and Physics Psychology and Neuroscience Science

RECENT POSTS

  • Biomimicry Within Bowdoin: The Ongoing Development of Peptoids April 4, 2023
  • The Possibilities of the CRISPR-Cas9 System April 2, 2023
  • The Chronic Lyme Debate April 2, 2023

FOLLOW US

  • Facebook
  • Twitter

Footer

TAGS

AI AI ethics antibiotics antidepressants bacteria Bathymetry BDA Biology birds Birth control Cell Biology Chlorofluorocarbons chronic lyme contaminants Cytoskeleton Death Prediction Diabetes dreams Ethics fossil fuels heterogeneity Immunology Immunotherapy kleptomania lyme disease marine Marine Biology Marine Mammals Marine noise Medicine mercury Montreal Protocol Moss neural network ocean Plants plastic REM seabirds serotonin SERT sleep textfakes ticks Women's health

Copyright © 2023 · students.bowdoin.edu